Scientists find an 'Alphabet' in whale songs
The First Art Newspaper on the Net    Established in 1996 Sunday, December 22, 2024


Scientists find an 'Alphabet' in whale songs
In an image provided by Amanda Cotton, a group of sperm whales, which do not produce the eerie melodies sung by humpback whales, but instead rattle off clicks that sound like a cross between Morse code and a creaking door. In an article published in Nature Communications in May 2024, researchers assert that sperm whales are using a much richer set of sounds than previously known, which they called a “sperm whale phonetic alphabet.”

by Carl Zimmer



NEW YORK, NY.- Ever since the discovery of whale songs almost 60 years ago, scientists have been trying to decipher their lyrics. Are the animals producing complex messages akin to human language? Or sharing simpler pieces of information, like dancing bees do? Or are they communicating something else we don’t yet understand?

In 2020, a team of marine biologists and computer scientists joined forces to analyze the click-clacking songs of sperm whales, the gray, block-shaped leviathans that swim in most of the world’s oceans. On Tuesday, the scientists reported that the whales use a much richer set of sounds than previously known, which they called a “sperm whale phonetic alphabet.”

People have a pho-ne-tic alphabet too, which we use to produce a practically infinite supply of words. But Shane Gero, a marine biologist at Carleton University in Ottawa, Ontario, and an author of the study, said it’s unclear whether sperm whales similarly turn their phonetic sounds into a language.

“The fundamental similarities that we do find are really fascinating,” Gero said. “It’s totally changed the way we have to do work going forward.”

Since 2005, Gero and his colleagues have followed a clan of 400 sperm whales around Dominica, an island nation in the eastern Caribbean, eavesdropping on the whales with underwater microphones and tagging some of the animals with sensors.

Sperm whales don’t produce the eerie melodies sung by humpback whales, which became a sensation in the 1960s. Instead, they rattle off clicks that sound like a cross between Morse code and a creaking door. Sperm whales typically produce pulses of between three and 40 clicks, known as codas. They usually sing these codas while swimming together, raising the possibility that they’re communicating with one another.

Over the years, Gero and his colleagues have reviewed thousands of hours of recordings of the undersea noise. It turns out that sperm whale codas fall into distinct types.

One type, for example, called “1+1+3,” consists of two clicks separated by a pause, followed by three clicks in quick succession.

With backing from philanthropists, Gero and his colleagues started “Project CETI,” (for “Cetacean Translation Initiative”), to investigate whether artificial intelligence and other computing advances could decode whale songs. (The name is a play on SETI, the famous effort to search for extraterrestrial life; whales are also known as cetaceans.)

As part of the project, Pratyusha Sharma, a computer science graduate student at Massachusetts Institute of Technology, gave the data from Dominica a fresh look. But she was frustrated by the way biologists had visualized it.

On a computer screen, the codas appeared as a series of dots along a horizontal line, each dot representing a click. Sharma found it hard to compare codas, especially when two or more whales were singing over one another. So she instead plotted each coda’s clicks as dots on a vertical line, then placed codas along a horizontal line based on when each began.

Using the new layout, Sharma saw something new. When a sperm whale repeated a coda, it sometimes stretched out the time between the clicks and then gradually tightened it up. Sharma and her colleagues called this phenomenon “rubato,” a musical term for speeding up a tempo and then slowing it down.

Gero was startled that Sharma could see something in sperm whale songs that he and his colleagues had missed for years. “It was a way we hadn’t looked at it,” he said.

Codas are so quick that the human ear can miss a rubato. But the researchers found the pattern in thousands of recorded codas.

The researchers believe that rubato plays an important role in whale communication. They found that after one whale used rubato, neighboring whales would rapidly match the tempo change with their own codas.

Sharma’s new visualizations also revealed that sperm whales could occasionally add an extra click to the end of the coda, a behavior they call ornamentation. The scientists found evidence that the extra clicks were not just pointless flourishes. The whales that led groups often used ornamentation, after which their followers often responded with codas of their own.

The analysis showed that the conventional catalog of sperm whale codas could not capture their full complexity. Sperm whales can produce a 1+1+3 coda, for example, that lasts four-fifths of a second, or one second, or 1.25 seconds. Other codas may last only one-third of a second or half a second.

All told, the researchers identified 156 different codas, each with distinct combinations of tempo, rhythm, rubato and ornamentation. Gero said that this variation is strikingly similar to the way humans combine movements in our lips and tongue to produce a set of phonetic sounds.

A single sound like “ba” or “na” carries no semantic meaning on its own. But we can combine them into meaningful words like “banana.” The researchers raised the possibility that sperm whales might combine features of codas to convey meaning in a similar way.

Other experts said the whale alphabet marked an exciting advance. But they said sperm whale codas might be more akin to music than language.

“Music can have a strong influence on emotions without it actually conveying information,” said Taylor Hersh, a bioacoustician at Oregon State University. Rubato might be one way for sperm whales to tighten their social bonds, she speculated, by matching their songs.

Jacob Andreas, a computer scientist at MIT and an author of the study, said that the alphabet is allowing the researchers to dig deeper into whale songs. “Now we have gotten the machinery in place to start tackling the much more ambitious, long-term goal for Project CETI, which is trying to figure out what all of this actually means.”

Microphones deployed in the Caribbean are capturing ocean sounds 24 hours a day, and scientists are programming computers to learn how to pick sperm whale songs out from the background noise.

Andreas and his colleagues are also training artificial intelligence programs similar to ChatGPT. After listening to the sperm whale songs, these models might learn to recognize not just rubato and ornamentation, but other features that scientists have missed.

The hope is that computers will then be able to compose whale songs of their own, which could then be played to the whales.

That effort leaves other experts skeptical. Luke Rendell, a marine biologist at the University of St Andrews, in Scotland, worries that the AI models assume whale songs are a kind of language, rather than something more like music.

“I’ve no doubt that you could produce a language model that could learn to produce sperm whalelike sequences,” Rendell said. “But that’s all you get.”

This article originally appeared in The New York Times.










Today's News

May 9, 2024

A land artist's work evades demolition

Christie's announces 'Collection Crémieux, passion privée' sale in Paris

5.01-carat Type IIa Diamond brings $175,000 at Heritage, leads Spring Jewelry Auction to $4.7 million

At SFMOMA, disability artwork makes history

Cocktail hour at the Met Gala: What comes after the carpet

How much money did this year's Met Gala raise?

They put a 65-foot hot dog in Times Square, and it's a blast

World's first desktop computer leads other landmarks in Heritage's Video Game Auction

Klassik Stiftung Weimar opens "Bauhaus and National Socialism"

Is the Met Gala protest-proof?

After the Met Gala, the parties lasted all night

The theatrical dimension of Jordi Colomer takes over MACBA

Freeman's / Hindman to offer over 750 lots of fine art in four collecting categories in May

Lesley Hazleton, writer who tackled religion and fast cars, dies at 78

Ahlers & Ogletree announces results of Spring Jewelry & Timepieces sale

Kistefos Museum announces Tatiana Trouvé as 2024 Sculptor of the Year

Exhibition brings together four recent series by Al Qadiri

Sale will offer an exquisite array of Spanish and Italian art and furnishings

A who's who of the Los Angeles art world bids farewell to a champion

Scientists find an 'Alphabet' in whale songs

CCS Bard to double capacity of library and archives with new Keith Haring Wing

Arnaldo Pomodoro. Il movimento possibile on view at Cortesi Gallery

Make time for Rolexes and Omegas commemorating transfer of Panama Canal at Heritage's Watches & Fine Timepieces Auction

12 Ways to Stay Healthy During Taking Online Class




Museums, Exhibits, Artists, Milestones, Digital Art, Architecture, Photography,
Photographers, Special Photos, Special Reports, Featured Stories, Auctions, Art Fairs,
Anecdotes, Art Quiz, Education, Mythology, 3D Images, Last Week, .

 



Founder:
Ignacio Villarreal
(1941 - 2019)
Editor & Publisher: Jose Villarreal
(52 8110667640)

Art Director: Juan José Sepúlveda Ramírez
Writer: Ofelia Zurbia Betancourt

Attorneys
Truck Accident Attorneys
Accident Attorneys
Houston Dentist
Abogado de accidentes
สล็อต
สล็อตเว็บตรง
Motorcycle Accident Lawyer

Royalville Communications, Inc
produces:

ignaciovillarreal.org juncodelavega.com facundocabral-elfinal.org
Founder's Site. Hommage
to a Mexican poet.
Hommage
       

The First Art Newspaper on the Net. The Best Versions Of Ave Maria Song Junco de la Vega Site Ignacio Villarreal Site Parroquia Natividad del Señor
Tell a Friend
Dear User, please complete the form below in order to recommend the Artdaily newsletter to someone you know.
Please complete all fields marked *.
Sending Mail
Sending Successful